Accès gratuit
Numéro
Perspectives Psy
Volume 56, Numéro 4, octobre-décembre 2017
Page(s) 308 - 319
Section Histoire et actualité des troubles autistiques : diversité des approches (2e partie)
DOI https://doi.org/10.1051/ppsy/2017564308
Publié en ligne 18 mai 2018
  1. Aitken K.J., Trevarthen C. (1997). Self/other organization in human psychological development. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 653-677. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baron Cohen S., Leslie A.M., Frith U. (1985). Does the Autistic Child Have a Theory of Mind? Cognition, 21 (1), 37-46. [Google Scholar]
  3. Baron-Cohen S. (1995). Mindblindness: an essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  4. Baron-Cohen S. (2009). Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. Cambridge Mass : MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
  5. Baron-Cohen S., Tager-Flusberg H., Cohen D. (Eds.) (1993). Understanding other minds: Perspectives from Autism. Oxford : Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bolte S., Hubl D., Feineis-Matthews S., et al. (2006). Facial affect recognition training in autism: can we animate the fusiform gyrus? Behavioral Neuroscience, 120, 211-216. [Google Scholar]
  7. Castelli F., Frith C., Happé F. & Frith U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125, 1839-849. [Google Scholar]
  8. Chamak B. (2015). Le concept de neurodiversité ou l’éloge de la différence. In « Regards croisés sur l’idée de guérison et de rétablissement en santé mentale » (Déchamp-Leroux, Rafael Dir), John Libbey eurotext, 41-49, 978-2-7420-1422-4. [Google Scholar]
  9. Damasio A.R., Maurer R. (1978). A neurological model for childhood autism. Archives of Neurology, 35, 777-786. [Google Scholar]
  10. Dapretto M., Davies M.S., Pfeifer J.H., Scott A.A., Sigman M., Bookheimer S., Iacoboni M., (2006). Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 28-30. [Google Scholar]
  11. Decety J., Hodges S.D. (2005). The social neuroscience of empathy. In Lange P.A. (Ed) bridging social psychology: benefits of transdisciplinary approaches. Lawrence Erlbaum As. Nahwah NJ. [Google Scholar]
  12. Dinstein I., Cibu T., Humphreys K. & al (2010). Normal movement selectivity in autism. Neuron, 66 (3), 461-469. [Google Scholar]
  13. Evans B. (2013). How autism became autism: The radical transformation of a central concept of child development in Britain. History of the Human Sciences, 26 (3), 3-31. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Eyal G. (2010). The autism matrix. The social origins of autism epidemic. Polity Press, Cambridge. [Google Scholar]
  15. Frith C. & Frith U. (1999). Interacting minds – a biological basis. Science, 286, 1692-1695. [Google Scholar]
  16. Frith C.D. (2007). The social brain? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 362, 671-678. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  17. Frith U. (2003). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford : Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
  18. Frith U., Frith C.D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 358, 459-473. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  19. Frith U., Happé F. (1994). Autism: beyond “theory of mind”. Cognition, 50, 115-132. [Google Scholar]
  20. Gallese V., Rochat M.J., Berchio C. (2013). The mirror mechanism and its potential role in autism spectrum disorder. Developemental Medicine of Child Neurology, 55 (1), 15-22. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gallese V. (2007). Before and below “theory of mind”: embodied simulation and the neural correlates of social cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 362, 659-669. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  22. Georgieff N (2010). Psychanalyse, neurosciences et subjectivités. Neuropsychiatrie de l’enfance et de l’adolescence, 58 (6/7), 343-50. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  23. Georgieff N. (2014). Intersubjectivité : une perspective développementale. Enfance et psy, 1 (62), 50-56. [Google Scholar]
  24. Georgieff N., Jeannerod M. (1998). Beyond consciousness of external reality. A “who” system for consciousness of action and self-consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 7, 465-477. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Georgieff N., Speranza M. (2013). Psychopathologie de l’Intersubjectivité. Masson, Paris. [Google Scholar]
  26. Gergely G. (2001). The obscure object of desire: “Nearly, but clearly not me”. Contingency preference in normal children versus children with autism. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, Vol. 65 Cognitive and interactional foundations of attachment, 411-426. [Google Scholar]
  27. Gillberg C.L. (1992). The Emmanuel Miller Lecture 1991: Autism and autistic-like conditions: subclasses among disorders of empathy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33 (5), 813-842. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  28. Grandin T. (1997). Penser en images et autres témoignages sur l’autisme. O. Jacob, Paris. [Google Scholar]
  29. Griffith E., Pennington B., Wehner E., Rogers S. (1999). Executive functions in young children with autism. Child Development, 70, 817-832. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Guedes Neta M.L., Varanda C.A. (2016). The role of mirror neurons in autism spectrum disorders. Austin Journal of Autism and Related Disabilities. [Google Scholar]
  31. Hamilton A.F. (2013). Reflecting on the mirror neuron system in autism: A systematic review of current theories. Developmental Cognitive Neurosciences, 3, 91-105. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  32. Hamilton AF. (2008). Emulation and mimicry for social interaction: a theoretical approach to imitation in autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 101-15. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Happé F. (1999). Autism: cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 216-222. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Hickok G. (2009). Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of action understanding in mokeys and humans. Journal of Cognitive Neurosciences, 21 (7), 1229-1243. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  35. Hobson R.P. (1993). Autism and the development of mind. Hillsdale, NJ : Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  36. Hochmann J. (2015). Histoire des antipsychiatries. O. Jacob, Paris. [Google Scholar]
  37. Hochmann J. (2013). « Histoire de l’autisme ». O. Jacob, Paris. [Google Scholar]
  38. Hollin G. (2014). Constructing a social subject: autism and human sociality in the 1980s. History of the Human Sciences, 27 (4), 98-115. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  39. Iacoboni M., Dapretto M. (2006). The mirror neuron system and the consequences of its dysfunction. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 942-951. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  40. Jeannerod M. (2006). Motor Cognition. Oxford University Press. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  41. Jones W, Klin A. (2013). Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2-6 month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature, 504, 427-431. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Just M., Keller T., Malave V. et al. (2012). Autism as a neural systems disorder: A theory of frontalposterior underconnectivity. Neurosciences and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36 (4), 1292-1313. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Klin A., Schulz S., Jones W. (2015). Social visual engagements in infants and toddlers with autism: early developmental transitions and a model of pathogenesis. Neurosciences and Biobehavioral Reviews 50, 189-203. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  44. Klin A., Jones W., Schultz R., Volkmar F. & Cohen D. (2002). Defining and quantifying the social phenotype in autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 895-908. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  45. Klin A., Sparrow S., de Bildt A., Cicchetti D., Cohen D., Volkmar, F. (1999). A normed study of face recognition in autism and related disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 499-508. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Klin A., Volkmar F., Sparrow S. (1992). Autistic social dysfunction: Some limitations of the theory of mind. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 861-876. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  47. Leslie A.M. (1987). Pretense and representation: the origins of autism, and the “Theory-of-Mind”. Psychological Review 94 (4) : 412-426. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  48. Markram K., & Markram H. (2010). The intense world theory – a unifying theory of the neurobiology of autism. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 4, 224. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mottron L. (2006). L’autisme, une autre intelligence. Mardaga. [Google Scholar]
  50. Mottron L. (2016). L’Intervention précoce pour enfants autistes : nouveaux principes pour soutenir une autre intelligence. Mardaga. [Google Scholar]
  51. Mottron L., Duret P., Mueller S., & al (2015). Sex differences in brain plasticity: a new hypothesis for sex ratio bias in autism. Molecular Autism, 6, 33. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Mottron L., Dawson M., Soulières I., Hubert B., Burack J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: an update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 27-43. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Mukamel R., Ekstrom A.D., Kaplan J. & al (2010). Single-Neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions. Current Biology, 20 (8), 750-756. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  54. Muratori F. (2012). L’autisme comme conséquence d’un trouble de l’intersubjectivité primaire. La Psychiatrie de l’Enfant, 1 (55), 41-82. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  55. Oberman L., Hubbard E.M., Mc Cleery J.P. & al (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 190-198. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  56. Oberman L.M., Ramachandran V.S. (2007). The simulating social mind: the role of the mirror neuron system and simulation in the social and communicative deficits of autism spectrum disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 2, 310-327. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Ozonoff S. (1997). Components of executive function in autism and other disorders. In : J. Russell (Ed.), Autism as an executive disorder (pp. 179-211). Oxford : Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  58. Pennington, B. (2006). From single to multiple deficit models of developmental disorders. Cognition, 101, 385-413. [Google Scholar]
  59. Rizzolati G., Fabbri-Destro M. (2010). Mirror neurons: from discovery to autism. Experimental Brain Research, 200, 223-237. [Google Scholar]
  60. Rizzolatti G., Craighero L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169-192. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Rochat P. (2009). Others in mind: social origins of self-consciousness. Cambridge. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  62. Russell J. (1997). Autism as an executive disorder. Oxford : Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  63. Schulte-Rüther M., Otte E., Adigüzel K. & al (2016). Intact mirror mechanisms for automatic facial emotions in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Research, [Google Scholar]
  64. Schunke O., Schottle D., Vettorazzi E. & al (2016). Mirror me: Imitative responses in adults with autism. Autism, 20 (2), 134-144. [Google Scholar]
  65. Sigman M. (1998). The Emanuel Miller Memorial Lecture 1997: Change and continuity in the development of children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 879-891. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  66. Smith A. (2009). The Empathy Imbalance Hypothesis of Autism: A Theoretical Approach to Cognitive and Emotional Empathy in Autistic Development. The Psychological Record ; Heidelberg 59.2 (Spring 2009) : 273-294. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  67. Stern D. (2005). Le désir d’intersubjectivité. Pourquoi ? Comment ? Psychothérapies, 4, 25. [Google Scholar]
  68. Tammet D. (2009). Embrasser le ciel immense. Éditions des Arènes. [Google Scholar]
  69. Trevarthen C., Aitken K.J. (2003). Intersubjectivité chez le nourrisson : recherche, théorie et application clinique. Devenir, 15 (4), 309-428. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  70. Trevarthen C., Delafield-Butt J.T. (2013). Autism as a developmental disorder in intentional movement and affective engagement. Frontier in Integrative Neurosciences 17, 7, 49. [Google Scholar]
  71. Valeri V., Speranza M. (2009). Modèles neuropsychologiques dans l’autisme et les troubles envahissants du édveloppement. Développements, 1, 34-38. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  72. Vivanti G., Rogers S. J. (2014). Autism and the mirror neuron systems: insights from learning and teaching. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B. 369, 1644. [Google Scholar]
  73. Wicker B., Fonlupt P., Hubert B. (2008). Abnormal cerebral effective connectivity during explicit emotional processing in adults with autism spectrum disorder. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 3 (2), 135-143. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Widlöcher D. (1994). Traité de Psychopathologie. P.U.F., Paris. [Google Scholar]
  75. Williams D. (1992). Si on me touche, je n’existe plus. Laffont. [Google Scholar]
  76. Williams J.H.G., Whiten A., Suddendorf T, Perret D.I. (2017). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. [Google Scholar]
  77. Williams J.H.G. (2008). Self-other relations in social development and autism: multiple roles for mirror neurons and other brain bases. Autism Research, 2, 73-90. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  78. Williams J.H.G., Whiten A., Suddendorf T. & Perrett D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 287-295. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Les statistiques affichées correspondent au cumul d'une part des vues des résumés de l'article et d'autre part des vues et téléchargements de l'article plein-texte (PDF, Full-HTML, ePub... selon les formats disponibles) sur la platefome Vision4Press.

Les statistiques sont disponibles avec un délai de 48 à 96 heures et sont mises à jour quotidiennement en semaine.

Le chargement des statistiques peut être long.